Originally published in slightly different form on October 14, 2010 at PsychologyToday.com.
Dogs are the most diverse
species of animal on earth. According to zoologist Desmond Morris, there are
over 1,000 breeds, each with its own specific body type and character traits.
Meanwhile, even though the
latest genetic tests show that dogs evolved from wolves, there is nothing in
the behavior or morphology of the domesticated dog’s wild cousins to account
for the dog’s incredible physical and behavioral diversity. So where did it
come from?
Up until recently it was
thought that when our ancestors bred dogs for the qualities of tameness,
submission, and sociability, we were also, either accidentally or
intentionally, increasing their genetic tendencies toward neoteny
(a form of pedomorphosis),
i.e., the retention of juvenile characteristics—both physical and behavioral—into
adulthood. And these pedomorphisms resulted in variegated coats, differences in
morphology, and the kinds of behavioral characteristics found in juvenile
wolves.
The theory of neoteny was
reinforced by the famous Russian silver fox experiments done at the Institute of Cytology and
Genetics in Siberia under the direction of geneticist Dmitry K.
Belyaev.
Belyaev was asked by some
Russian fur traders if he could create a tame silver fox that
wouldn’t bite or be afraid of being handled by humans.
He succeeded. But within a
few generations, the foxes began to show other neotenous traits, wagging tails,
barking. And while they exhibited no fear of humans, their coats became
variegated and mottled, which decreased the value of their fur to “nulyu”
(Russian for zero).
In 2002 Raymond and Lorna
Coppinger presented a new model for domestication, based partly on Belyaev’s
work, and partly on observations of modern wolves, some of whom scavenge at
human garbage dumps. Their model stated that wolves (or early dogs), were
attracted to the garbage at our ancestors’ encampments. And the animals who
were the boldest, i.e., most unafraid of our human ancestors, and had the least
tendency to bite, were slowly welcomed into our human packs. Over time, these
bold, non-biting proto-doggies, became our closest companions.
However, since the trait
of “dominance” has been equated with being able to control resources, and since
the boldest proto-dogs would’ve had more control over who, within in their
specific family groups, were able to gain access to the human garbage, there
are still two unanswered questions. How does selecting for dominance/boldness
translate into the opposite, and supposedly neotenous, trait of submission? And
if our ancestors were breeding for the neotenous quality of submissiveness, why
do so many dogs still exhibit dominant tendencies?
There may be another
problem. While the Coppingers’ “dogs-as-scavengers” theory, and Belayaev’s work
with foxes are important steps in our current understanding of canine
evolution, they’re still based in large part on the idea that diversity in dogs
is solely (or primarily) the result of human intervention, having little to do
with some inherent quality found in the dog/wolf/(fox) gene pool.
New genetic research may
change that. Evolutionary biologist Ursula Goodenough wrote
about this research recently for NPR. “Breeding programs,” she writes, “can
only yield as much variation as is harbored in the gene pool, and the dog gene
pool [is] a gold mine.”
So what’s inside this gold
mine?
Two things: insertional
mutations, and tandem-repeat sequences.
Goodenough: “Dog genomes
harbor DNA sequences, called mobile SINEC_Cf elements, that tend to leave one
chromosomal location and insert themselves into a second ... Should they happen
to insert into a gene or a regulatory element, this modifies the encoded
genetic information, generating ‘insertional mutations.’ It’s been estimated
that dog genomes have at least 11,000 potentially mobile SINE elements whereas
human genomes have less than 1,000.”
The second source of
Fido’s diversity comes during fetal development. That’s because dog DNA also
carries a feature called “tandem-repeat sequences,” where one bit of genetic
code is repeated over and over (and over and over). “When such regions are
copied in the germ line,” Goodenough says, “the copying enzymes tend to ...
synthesize too many or too few repeats, generating ‘slippage mutations’ in eggs
and sperm that are inherited by offspring. The slippage rate [is] far higher in
dogs than in other carnivores.”
The next question is, at
what point did the insertional mutations and slippage rates in the dog’s DNA
come into play? Was it during the domestication process, or were they already
there, lying in wait somewhere inside the wolf’s DNA?
We know that all wolves
exhibit the same basic morphology, species-wide, with only fairly minor
variations in size, color, coat density, etc. So on first glance it seems very
unlikely that they would possess the genetic blueprint necessary for all the
variations we see in domesticated dogs. Yet it turns out that the DNA of modern
wolves also shows the same high levels of the two mutational factors. So
apparently, dogs did, in fact, inherit their diversity and variability directly
from wolves.
What does this have to
do with a unified dog theory?
For the past six or seven
years, key figures in the positive training movement have been dismissing or
denigrating the dog’s genetic history. Dr. Ian Dunbar, a key figurehead in the
movement has said, “Learning from wolves to interact with pet dogs makes about
as much sense as, ‘I want to improve my parenting—let’s see how the chimps do
it!’“ (This is based on a comparative DNA analysis between dogs and wolves in
relation to roughly the same percentage of DNA shared by humans and chimps.)
While this idea makes
sense on a certain level, there’s such a huge divide between humans and our
closest genetic relatives, that there’s really no comparison to dogs and
wolves. It’s true that chimps are much smarter than we previously thought, but
they still don’t design and construct buildings, they don’t play guitar, write
poetry, build airplanes, hold elections, take summer vacations, shop for shoes,
etc.
So yes, there are
differences between dogs and wolves, but it’s not as big a divide as the one
that exists between humans and chimps. And the key differences exist primarily
because of the dog’s relationship with humans, a relationship that for
thousands of years was based, not on the dog’s talent for scavenging, but on
how our ancestors used the dog’s hunting instincts, which were directly
inherited from the wolf.
So while the Coppinger
model of domestication may be valid, at least in part, and while neoteny is
surely a factor in terms of how dogs fit in to a human household, neither model
can successfully explain how dogs-as-scavengers ended up being herders,
pointers, ratters, sighthounds, scenthounds, retrievers, and on and on.
Other questions remain.
For instance, do foxes have these same mutational qualities in their DNA? If
so, are they found in the same levels as in dogs and wolves? (My hunch is that
foxes have them, just at lower levels). After all, foxes are the only canid who
don’t hunt large prey by forming packs.1
And the only way the pack style of
hunting can be successful is through variability in temperament, relative to
each animal’s role in the hunt: some need to be bold while others need to hold
back, or circle the prey, etc. That’s why in every pack, some wolves have a direct
(or dominant) temperament, while others are more indirect (or submissive). If
they all had the same approach to prey, the hunt would surely fail.
This diversity of
temperament can also be seen in every litter of puppies, from great Danes to
Chihuahuas, a clear spectrum from the most direct (or “dominant”) to the most
indirect (or “submissive”). And with all the different kinds of breeding
programs humans have devised over the last 14,000+ years, why does this
diversity—this spectrum from dominant to submissive—still show itself with
such clarity and exactitude in each and every litter of puppies born to every
female dog, in every breed imaginable?
It has to be related to
the prey drive. So either evolution hasn’t been paying attention to the fact that
dogs no longer need to hunt large prey to survive, or else there’s something
else contained within the wolf’s style of hunting that’s vital to the
domesticated dog’s relationship with us. After all, homo sapiens and some
members of the canidae family are the only two types of land animal with an
evolutionary history of hunting large, dangerous prey by working in concert.
Most of us don’t think of
ourselves or our dogs as predators. But glimmers of that early relationship
still show themselves in our daily lives. Whether we’re taking the dog for a
walk (the search), or tossing a Frisbee (the chase), or laughing at the way a
puppy “snaps the neck” of her new toy (the kill bite), underneath it all, dogs
and humans share an emotional bond that goes back 14,000 years or more.
There’s no getting around
the fact that no matter how evolved we are, both species still have aggressive
tendencies, emotions, and impulses. Human aggression is usually expressed
through playing (or watching) sports, or else it’s sublimated into our work, or
stimulated and released by certain types movies. Dogs offload their aggression
into toys and games. And in order to live in harmony with others, each of us—dog
and human—needs a safe means of offloading aggression.
So despite the very
sensible and rational idea that we don’t need to know anything about the wolf
in order to train our dogs, it seems to me that understanding where a
dog’s aggressive nature comes from, and knowing how to redirect that aggressive
energy into a safe outlet through play, may be one of the most important things
we can do for our dogs, and for ourselves.
Next time: Understanding
the Real Wolf Model.
“Life Is an Adventure—Where Will Your Dog Take You?”
Join Me on Facebook!
Follow Me on Twitter!
Join the Rescue Dog Owners Support Group!
Join Me on Facebook!
Follow Me on Twitter!
Join the Rescue Dog Owners Support Group!
Footnotes:
1) Cape hunting dogs (the
domesticated dog’s most distant relative) hunt in packs, although their hunting
style is based on sheer weight of numbers, which is not related to the wolf’s
chase-and-ambush style, a style that coyotes (and sometimes dingoes and
jackals) use when they need to hunt large prey.
No comments:
Post a Comment