“It is difficult to resist the idea that general principles underlie non-hierarchical systems, such as ant colonies and brains. And because organizations without hierarchy are unfamiliar, broad analogies between systems are reassuring. But the hope that general principles will explain the regulation of all the diverse complex dynamical systems that we find in nature, can lead to ignoring anything that doesn’t fit a pre-existing model.”—Deborah M. Gordon “Control Without Hierarchy,” (Nature, March, 2007.)
Prologue by LCK
I
believe that the concept of dominance hierarchies in animal groups—particularly
as it’s applied to dogs and wolves—is long overdue for the scientific scrap
heap. Yet it persists.
Primatologist Thelma Rowell—who studied
baboons, and whose observations overturned much of what was known about their
social behaviors at the time—, felt that hierarchies should be labeled as
subordinance or even “stress hierarchies.” (1974.)
She was ahead of her time (and still is).
In most animal hierarchies, the most dominant
member generally produces the most stress hormone, cortisol. Of course, like
many hormones, cortisol has several modes of operation. For instance, prolonged
elevated levels lead to muscle wasting, and there doesn’t seem to be any
evidence of that in alpha wolves (at least not while they’re young).
Another potential problem—as I see it—is that
the social behaviors of animals have long been described through the principles
of economics and game theory, as if a wolf pack, eg., were a market system or a game. This is why we see
scientific papers about “the division of labor” and “cheating” in wolf packs.
However, I believe that a better model might
be to see the pack as an information system. If we apply that concept to alpha
wolves—incorporating the seemingly contradictory ideas that these animals
produce more cortisol yet seemingly show no ill effects from elevated levels—we
might surmise that cortisol acts as a form of information, and that more
dominant wolves may have more carrying capacity than other members of the
group. After all, so-called alpha wolves are generally in the position of
having to process more information about their environment than their
subordinates are; being a leader means having to focus one’s attention on far
more details.
Another possible way to reinvent or replace
the alpha model has been proposed by veteran dog trainer and natural philosopher Kevin Behan. In the
following guest post, he suggests that we look at the pack as a flow system,
based on Dr. Adrian Bejan’s Constructal
Law: “For a finite system to
persist in time (to live) it must evolve in a way that provides easier access
to the imposed currents that flow through it.”
Here are some of Kevin’s thoughts on the problem of dominance in dogs.
Shifting Stands,
Shifting Sands
The theory of dominance has shifted over the 50 years or so that I’ve been a dog trainer. It used to be about social superiority. Every individual was thought to be endowed with an inborn impulse to dominate others, as well as a counterbalancing impulse to submit once the dust had settled. A competitive struggle sorted this into a hierarchy of rank with a “top dog,” “alpha personality,” or “leader of the pack” at the peak of the pyramid.
The theory of dominance has shifted over the 50 years or so that I’ve been a dog trainer. It used to be about social superiority. Every individual was thought to be endowed with an inborn impulse to dominate others, as well as a counterbalancing impulse to submit once the dust had settled. A competitive struggle sorted this into a hierarchy of rank with a “top dog,” “alpha personality,” or “leader of the pack” at the peak of the pyramid.
The problem is that sometimes an inferior
animal is able to control the behavior of its superiors by controlling access
to certain resources. In this new way of looking at social behavior, a dominant
individual doesn’t achieve status, it achieves access. And no individual is
inherently dominant or submissive, rather there is a spectrum of “personality
types.”
Emergence theory has also
been applied to hierarchies in animal groups. In emergence theory, each
relationship is determined by a local set of circumstances independent of the
larger matrix of interactions. Dominance and submission emerge from such
relationships as opposed to being some inherent, genetic quality contained
within each animal.
The Bold and the
Dutiful
A
good summation of this new definition can be found in an online article
entitled "Why Won't Dominance Die?"
written by former police-dog trainer, David Ryan. It was written to discredit Cesar Millan’s
approach to dog training.
In it, Ryan talks about the concept of
dominance in dogs as a “meme,” a word coined by
biologist Richard Dawkins to describe self replicating ideas that inhabit our
minds and get passed along from one individual to another as if they were
cultural viruses or genes. In Ryan’s view the dominance theory of dog training
is a harmful meme, and like a super-virus, it’s extremely resistant to
extinction.
“The concept of ‘dominance,’” Ryan says, “has
never been a quality of an individual, but the product of a relationship.
Ethologists label an animal dominant over another once there is a trend towards
the second animal deferring in encounters between the two.”
He goes on to say that there are two types of
dog, the bold and the shy, and that a “smooth relationship [between the two] is
one in which each knows the other’s preferences and defers accordingly.”
Meet the New Boss, Same as the Old Boss
I
would argue that we’ll never be able to replace the old notion of dominance
with the one Ryan proposes because they’re essentially based on the same
underlying “meme.” If dominance is about access to a resource (so that social
life is not a constant struggle for status), don't all individuals crave
access? Isn't it better to end up at the dominant end of a relationship and
thereby enjoy unfettered access to resources?
Obviously yes. So the constant struggle for
social ascendancy is merely being replaced by a constant struggle over
resources. Of course the new school says that, no, social life isn’t a constant
struggle because the dominant/subordinate relationship ameliorates aggression.
But the old model said the exact same thing, and still does.
Plus, if dogs vary in terms of bold vs. shy
(as opposed to dominant vs. submissive), how is a shy (i.e., inhibited)
individual ever going to gain control over a resource?
It turns out that a shy dog can attain
dominance by getting to the resource first, possession being “9/10ths of the
law.” And, once in possession, a shy dog becomes emboldened, while the bold dog—whose
access is now blocked—becomes more shy in nature. This suggests that control confers
boldness and that lack of access confers its polar opposite. Remember, in the
old definition, status confers, induces, or augments the trait of dominance. In
the new definition access is the controlling factor.
So I would argue that just saying dogs vary
in terms of bold versus shy fails to articulate the true dynamic from which the
relationship emerges just as the old definition failed to do so.
The Problem With
Cesar Millan
Finally,
Ryan’s piece was directed at Cesar Millan, our most famous proponent of the
dominance model of dog training. But in Ryan’s critique what exactly is the
beef? According to this new definition, Cesar is doing it right 99% of the
time. (We should discount the really rough stuff because Cesar would argue
these are last ditch cases about to be euthanized and represent but a small
portion of his methodology even though they occupy a disproportionate share of
the viewing time).
Cesar explicitly argues for a subtle
manipulation of the innate desire within a dog to please its “pack leader,”
along with massive doses of exercise. What’s wrong with that?
Of course Cesar is behind the times. He
should be calling himself the pack parent rather than the pack leader. But he’s
on solid behavioral ground according to both the new and old definitions of
dominance. He controls a dog’s access to every resource and therefore he “emerges”
as the dominant in this context, the dominant in that context, the dominant in all
contexts.
He may be mistaken about a hierarchal pack
leader, he may not be able to articulate that dogs are in a constant struggle
for access to resources as opposed to social ascendancy, etc. But if the dog
sees him as being in control of every resource then, operationally-speaking,
what’s the difference? Cesar’s belief in his role as pack leader emerges from
the social construct he engineers, and it’s engineered in accordance with the
modern, accepted definition of dominance.
This is why I think Ryan and others will find
it impossible to replace the old meme with this new one since they both have
the same two fundamentals in common, a) control over another’s behavior, and b)
the idea that sociability is about competition not cooperation.
In other words, dog owners are still being
taught to see their dogs as rivals not partners.
Dominance and
Submission as Forms of Flow?
I
suggest we turn to Dr. Adrian Bejan’s book, Design In Nature, to help us see hierarchy from a new perspective.
In it Bejan, argues that nature does not work according to principles of
control but principles of flow.
For instance, in Bejan’s view a forest is a
hierarchy of a few very large trees relative to many smaller forms of
vegetation. The various plants aren’t competing for light, water or nutrients.
The tallest ones aren’t trying to dominate the shorter ones. The forest simply
emerges as the most efficient way to conduct and improve the flows of all
currents contained within it (nutrients, air, water, stress). Each organism is
seen as an engine within a larger one, all contributing to improve of access
for all to the underlying currents. The hierarchy self-organizes not around
competition or cooperation, but around the current.
So instead of asking of dogs: “Who’s in
control of whom?” (old school) or “Who’s in control of what?” (new school) I
think we should be asking, “What is the current around which a dog’s social
structure emerges?” And I think if we look at canines without imposing
humanlike thoughts on to their behavior we might be able to see such a current
emerge.
A linear definition imbued with the notions
of control and competition cannot be made to conform to the principles of flow.
And I believe that only flow can accurately reflect the true workings of the
animal mind. Dominance is based on human thought processes, the comparison of
one abstraction relative to another, along with comparison of past, present,
and possible future moments in time. But flow—whether a flow of emotion or
information—is felt viscerally and unconsciously, and is always capable of
being apprehended in the now moment.
Kevin Behan
Natural Dog Training
NDT on Facebook!
Kevin Behan
Natural Dog Training
NDT on Facebook!
No comments:
Post a Comment