I don’t own an alarm clock. I
can’t remember the last time I had one let alone used it. Years ago I developed
an ability to “program” my subconscious mind to wake me up at any time I want,
within a range of about 5 - 10 minutes. Of course there are certain times when
the system fails due to a disagreement between my mind and body:
Lee’s Body: “He’s tired [or
sick, or was up very late], let him sleep!”
Lee’s Mind: “Well, he did say
that he wished he didn’t have to get up this early...”
However, back when my
Dalmatian Freddie was alive, he would always wake me up on time. Always. And I
never got a grace period. Even though dogs have no sense of time, Freddie
always woke me up at the exact moment I was supposed to get out of bed. He
didn’t do it by barking, or whining; he would just let out a heavier-than-usual
sigh, or lift up his head and shake it a few times, making his ears flap
loudly, or get up, circle around, maybe scratch at his bed a few times, then
lie down with a heavy thump and sigh. He seemed oblivious to what effects this
had on my sleep or wakefulness. In other words there didn’t seem to be any
intent on his part to wake me; yet he did it faithfully, every single time I
failed to wake up on my own. Now how strange and amazing is that?
Dog trainer and natural
philosopher Kevin Behan1
has written about a similar situation with his dog Lillo, a superbly-trained
German shepherd. Kevin had to be at an important meeting, forgot to set his
alarm clock, and Lillo, who had been trained to never come into the bedroom (I
think it was an issue with fleas and shag carpeting) did so anyway and woke
Kevin exactly when the alarm was supposed to have gone off.
One might suppose from this
that Lillo did a have conscious intent to wake Kevin. But why didn’t Freddie?
Kevin, who’s developed an energy theory
of animal behavior, would say that neither dog did. Instead they were each
influenced by changes in their own emotional state in relation to an unresolved
desire they felt in their owner. That created a need in the dog to move energy
out of his own system to reduce that feeling of pressure.
In Freddie’s case it
manifested in the atypically restless movements he made in and around his bed.
In Lillo’s case it was a much stronger feeling of magnetic attraction to the
one person in the world he’d been taught to always rely on to resolve such
feelings. (Also: Kevin’s desire was probably stronger, and therefore created
more displacement in Lillo’s emotional/energy field.)
To my way of thinking neither dog had a “reason”
for its behavior. They each felt an increase in emotional pressure and simply
tried to reduce it. Using Kevin’s theory I think it’s possible to describe all
canine behavior (perhaps all animal behavior) in terms of how emotional energy
flows or gets blocked.2
So when people tell me dogs
have the ability to reason, I say, “Hold on, let’s try to understand their
emotions first before we start giving them intellectual faculties.”
This brings me to the topic
at hand. For those who aren’t aware of it, I’m happy to report that Marc Bekoff recently joined Psychology
Today. If you don’t know the name, he’s a world-renowned expert on animal cognition,
someone who has keen, perhaps unparalleled insights into animal behavior, and
displays an almost astonishing affinity for animals of all kinds. Bekoff has
also expressed his desire to improve the lives of every animal on earth. I hope
I’m doing that myself, in my small role as a dog trainer. However, I have
to take exception to some of the things Bekoff has written in a recent
article here in which he sings the praises of anthropomorphism, and
suggests that by doing more of it we would also be showing more compassion to
and respect for animals.
First of all, I don’t think
we need to anthropomorphize dogs in order to show them respect. In some ways
they’re more social and civilized than we are.1
True, they have no knack for
literature, architecture, science, technology, or mass transportation, but they’re more openly social and cooperative
than the human race could ever hope to be. So let’s not start dragging them “up
to our level.”
However, I do agree 100% with
some of the points Bekoff makes. For one he says that the “privacy of mind”
concept has become outdated, and that with recent advances in various
scientific disciplines we can more readily and more easily understand animal
cognition. All true, but I would argue that this means we should no longer have
any need to anthropomorphize. Careful, objective analysis, using strong
theoretical frameworks, should tell us nearly everything we need to know.
Beckoff also posits that the
tendency to anthropomorphize is hardwired into our brains. Again, I agree, and
have found it’s more true of dogs than of any other species. (I wrote a
three-part article here on this very subject: 1) Dogs Have
Colonized Our Subconscious, 2) The Dog as
Psychotherapist, and 3) The Dog Who
Taught Me How to Forgive My Father.) Bekoff then tells us that we
should only do it the “right” way, which he calls “bio-centric
anthropomorophism.”
But with all due respect, can
we really have it both ways? If it’s hardwired then how much control can we
have over it? Once you let the genie out of the bottle a lot of people are
bound to come up with their own, very nasty ways of doing it.
The bestselling dog training
manual in the past 30 years is How to Be
Your Dog’s Best Friend by the Monks of New Skete. And their training
philosophy is based on two of the most harmful anthropomorphisms there are as
far as cruelty to dogs is concerned: the concepts of the pack leader,
and of dominance and
submission. On page 34 of the original edition of their book, the
monks write, “How hard should you hit your dog?” Their answer to this terrible
question is even worse: “If she doesn’t yelp in pain, you haven’t hit her hard
enough.”
Wow.
And the reason the monks
think (or once thought) this is okay is that they believe that if dogs perceive
us as higher up in the pack hierarchy, they’ll obey us. And when they don’t
obey, it’s because they “think” they’re alpha and need to be “taught a lesson.”
That’s one example of the
wrong kind of anthropomorphism. Another is that far too many people abandon
their dogs because they’re perceived as “stubborn,” “willful,” or “disobedient,”
all anthropomorphisms. The truth is, dogs can’t be obedient or disobedient
because even though it’s something they’re exceptionally good at, dogs
themselves don’t know what the concept of obedience means. They only know that
when their emotions are aligned with their owners’ desires they “feel” like
doing what their owners want them to. That’s all.
This brings up another point:
the primary reason people are unable to control their dogs to begin with is
that the literature on canine behavior is itself anthropomorphic to the
extreme. If people had a better sense of how and why dogs really behaved—that
they don’t do it to show respect to their “pack leader,” or because they’re
capable of propositional, if/then thinking (if I sit then I get a reward)—fewer
dogs would end up living in shelters and eventually being put to death. This is
another negative impact that anthropomorphism has on dogs.
Of course in his article
Bekoff is concerned with all animals, not just dogs, so here’s a final example,
unrelated to canines:
A woman in Connecticut
decides to keep a chimpanzee as a pet. And in the process of living with this
wonderful, loving, and intelligent animal she treats him as if he were her own
child. But he’s not a child, he’s a chimp. So a few years later, a terrible
tragedy takes place, leaving a neighbor woman blind and terribly
disfigured, and the poor young chimp dead. And that tragedy was the direct
result of anthropomorphism.
So I would argue that there
is no proper way to anthropomorphize animals just as there’s no proper way to
hit your dog.
I will agree that there are
discrete levels of consciousness which dogs and humans share. We’re both
influenced by certain instincts and drives, and we’re both strongly influenced
by emotion. Where I disagree is in the need to impute higher levels of
intellect onto animals. Don’t get me wrong. Marc Bekoff is more knowledgeable
and experienced than I am, so I could very well be missing something he’s
seeing that I’m not able to. But it seems to me that instead of artificially
(in my view) expanding animal consciousness vertically up the cognitive ladder
toward intellect, we should spend more time exploring how dogs themselves have
expanded their own social and emotional consciousness.
I’ve found that the more we
understand the real virtues of dogs, and extol and celebrate their true
dogginess, the safer and happier they’ll be. That should be everyone’s goal.
It’s clearly Bekoff’s; nothing could be clearer than that. And while it’s my
goal too, I think the best way to achieve it is to get down on the dog’s
level. I do it on a regular basis, and I can tell you firsthand it’s a pretty
cool place to be. Especially when you have to get up early and your alarm clock
won’t go off...
LCK
“Life Is an Adventure—Where Will Your Dog Take You?”
Join Me on Facebook!
Follow Me on Twitter!
Join the Rescue Dog Owners Support Group!
“Life Is an Adventure—Where Will Your Dog Take You?”
Join Me on Facebook!
Follow Me on Twitter!
Join the Rescue Dog Owners Support Group!
Footnotes:
1. I’ve called Kevin Behan a
“natural philosopher” because he strikes me as a dog-world combination of Newton, Darwin, and Freud.
2. Valerie Hunt, an Emeritus
Professor of Psychological Sciences at UCLA, has written that “Emotion is
aroused energy that takes a direction. Emotional energy is released whenever
there is action. It motivates us. When the emotion is strong and flowing there
is great physical energy. And when [it] is blocked weakness results.” She also
talks about the emotional field as follows:”Emotions can be viewed as a
computer program ... that directs information to all aspects of the mind and
body.”
3. Alexandra Semyonova did a
15-year study on the social behaviors of domesticated dogs (“The Social Organization of the Domestic Dog; A
Longitudinal Study of Domestic Canine Behavior and the Ontogeny of Canine
Social Systems,” Carriage House, The Hague.) In it she uses the
principles of autopoiesis (a form of emergence theory) to put forth the idea that dogs are
essentially a global-wide self-organizing system, interested in not just
maintaining their own “fitness hills” but in seeing that their behaviors don’t
have a negative impact on the fitness hills of other dogs. Her view is that
dogs who’ve gone through puppyhood free from undue emotional or physical trauma
are capable of countless harmonic social interactions with all other dogs in
all other situations. She also shows that aggression is considered an abnormal
behavior in dog society, whereas in terms of human history just the opposite is true.
No comments:
Post a Comment