Which Training Approach Is Best?
In
certain training circles it’s widely believed that learning theory is the only truly
scientific and, therefore, the only correct approach for training dogs.
Is
this true?
Not exactly.
To
be fair, learning theory (also known as behavioral science or behavior analysis)
is much more scientific than dominance theory, especially as it’s applied to
dog training. There is plenty of scientific evidence showing that dogs and wolves form
dominance hierarchies, but none showing that such hierarchies can cross species
boundaries.
Still,
while dominance training is based mostly on fantasy, not science, positive
training is not based on hard science. There are no underlying scientific
principles behind how learning theory works in the way that there are in
physics and chemistry, where natural phenomena are explained through specific scientific
laws. Learning theory is explained only through statistics. Don’t get me wrong.
Statistics are important. But they don’t describe the how and why of behavioral
changes in humans and animals, only the when and how often.
There
are two other problems. And they’re huge.
First,
modern, 21st Century research has shown that behavioral changes
don’t take place through associative learning, the theoretical process where a
human or animal associates a behavior with a reward and thus wants to repeat
that behavior to get another reward. etc. In fact, B. F. Skinner—who created this
model of learning—said that the only way to ensure that learning will stick is
to provide a series of initial rewards, then start withholding expected rewards
using what’s called a variable reinforcement ratio. And unless you have an
advanced degree, it can be very difficult to do this with
any accuracy.
So
that’s the first problem.
The second is that in actually studying the effects
that take place in the brains of animals during the learning process, scientists
have found a puzzling paradox: dopamine— often referred to as one of the
brain’s reward chemicals—is not released in connection with an external reward,
it’s released in the absence of an expected one. Other research shows that
dopamine is not really a reward chemical at all. It’s more like a salience
detector, meaning that it helps humans and animals pay close attention to
changing patterns in their environment. The upshot is that learning doesn’t
take place through forming an association between a behavior and a reward but
rather through a mostly unconscious process called pattern recognition.
I’ve
found that an easy way to determine if this is true is to teach a puppy to sit,
but do so, out of sequence, in the following manner: Show the puppy a treat. As
he tries to grab it, move it around in such a way that at some point the pup
sits on his own. As soon as he sits give him the treat. Then—after he’s already
eating his “reward”—wait a moment, then say “Sit!” in a happy voice.
Do
this three or four times. Then, show him the treat again and say “Sit!” without
waiting for him to exhibit the behavior. Wait a half-a-second or so, and the
pup will automatically sit.
Go
through this same process again later, in a slightly different environment, or
at a different time of day. And once you’ve done it several times, under
slightly different circumstances, all you have to do is show the pup a treat,
say “Sit!” and the pup will automatically sit.
If
learning takes place through associating a behavior with an external reward,
the pup wouldn’t learn to sit through this “backwards” way of doing things. Or
at the very least, it would take much longer for the pup to learn the behavior
and repeat it in a reliable way.
Of
course, the sit is among the easiest and most basic behaviors we can teach a
dog. And some readers may not see the distinction I’m making between associative
learning and pattern recognition. So let’s look at the very complex behaviors
exhibited by some working dogs.
Border
collies operate almost entirely on pattern recognition. They have to take into
account the movement of the flock as a whole, the behaviors of the individual
sheep, the changing nature of the terrain, the signals sent to them by the
shepherd, and a whole host of other factors. This is one reason why border
collies are among the “smartest” dogs. And it’s all due to pattern recognition.
There are very few, if any, external rewards provided when these dogs are
learning their trade and none while they’re working.
It’s
also well known that you can’t train police dogs, military dogs, bomb-sniffing
dogs, drug detection dogs, and search-and-rescue dogs using conditioning
techniques. That is, you can, you just
won’t get a good result if you try to do so.
Why?
It’s
well known that detection dogs will give false alerts in order to get a food
reward. Meanwhile, police dogs are never trained through rewards but through
games like tug-of-war that stimulate and satisfy the dog’s urge to bite, a form
of drive training that is quite different from reward-based techniques.
As
for search-and-rescue dogs—who often need to be able to navigate the rubble and
detritus of an urban disaster site—one training technique used is a scenario
where the dogs are urged to climb up or down fire escapes while their trainers
throw pots and pans in their direction. Talk about the opposite of positive
reinforcement! Yet doing this actually makes the dogs perform better.
There
many other examples where the behavioral science model and drive training
diverge.
The point is that when dogs are trained through elements of the wolf’s
prey drive—the search, the chase, the grab-bite, and the kill-bite—they learn
and operate through a completely different set of principles than those
espoused by reward-based trainers. And not only that, but the kind of learning
and obedience that takes place with drive training is far superior to what you
get with reward-based training.
Lee Charles Kelley
“Life Is an Adventure—Where Will Your Dog Take You?”
No comments:
Post a Comment